Language Disorders in Children Clinical Trial
— LK!2Official title:
Translating Research Into School-based Practice Via Small-group, Language-focused Comprehension Intervention
In the proposed project, the investigators will conduct a multisite randomized controlled trial (RCT) to determine the efficacy of Let's Know!2, a small-group, language focused comprehension intervention, on children's lower- and higher-level language skills and comprehension skills in the short- and long-term (Specific Aims 1 and 2). The investigators will also explore whether intervention effects are moderated by dosage, initial language skill, developmental language disorder (DLD) status, word reading skill, nonverbal IQ, and family socioeconomic status (Specific Aim 3). Children who have low language skills and are thus at risk for reading comprehension difficulties will participate in the study. Children will be randomly assigned to receive Let's Know! in small groups at their respective schools or to a business-as-usual control condition. The investigators will measure children's language and comprehension skills at the beginning and end of Grade 1 as well as in Grade 2 and Grade 3. The investigators hypothesize that children who experience Let's Know! will end Grade 1 with higher language skills than children in the control condition and that this will translate into better listening and reading comprehension skills as these children matriculate through elementary school.
Status | Recruiting |
Enrollment | 480 |
Est. completion date | December 31, 2025 |
Est. primary completion date | December 31, 2025 |
Accepts healthy volunteers | Accepts Healthy Volunteers |
Gender | All |
Age group | 5 Years to 12 Years |
Eligibility | Inclusion Criteria: - Parental consent to participate in the research; - Enrollment in Grade 1; - Scores below the OWL Language Screener - Basic English proficiency as reported by parents/teachers Exclusion Criteria: - Unable to speak or understand English at a basic level, as reported by parents/teachers; - Profound disability that severely impairs classroom participation, as reported by teachers; - Serious behavior issue that severely impairs classroom participation, as reported by teachers. |
Country | Name | City | State |
---|---|---|---|
United States | MGH Institute of Health Professions | Boston | Massachusetts |
United States | The Ohio State University | Columbus | Ohio |
Lead Sponsor | Collaborator |
---|---|
MGH Institute of Health Professions | National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD), Ohio State University, University of Kansas |
United States,
Adlof SM, Hogan TP. Understanding Dyslexia in the Context of Developmental Language Disorders. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2018 Oct 24;49(4):762-773. doi: 10.1044/2018_LSHSS-DYSLC-18-0049. Review. — View Citation
Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Little, T. D. (2006). Should the simple view of reading include a fluency component? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 933-958.
Alloway, T. P., & Gathercole, S. E. (2005). The role of sentence recall in reading and language skills of children with learning difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 271-282. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2005.05.001
Alonzo, C. N., Yeomans-Maldonado, G., Murphy, K. A., Bevens, B., & Language and Reading Research Consortium. (2016). Predicting second grade listening comprehension using prekindergarten measures. Topics in Language Disorders, 36, 312-333.
Baumann, J. F., Seifert-Kessell, N., & Jones, L. A. (1992). Effect of think-aloud instruction on elementary students' comprehension monitoring abilities. Journal of Reading Behavior, 24, 143-172.
Bengtson, E., Bridges, M., Daniels, D., Brandel, J., & Fisher, C. (2015). Improving language and listening skills in pre-kindergarten children during a summer literacy program. Poster session presented at KSHA, October.
Bridges, M.S., Piasta, S., Daniels, D., & Brandel, J. (February, 2016). Small-group intervention to support language and comprehension: Feasibility and Pilot Data. Presented at Pacific Coast Research Conference, San Diego, CA.
Castles A, Rastle K, Nation K. Ending the Reading Wars: Reading Acquisition From Novice to Expert. Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2018 Jun;19(1):5-51. doi: 10.1177/1529100618772271. Review. Erratum in: Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2018 Nov;19(2):93. — View Citation
Catts HW, Adlof SM, Ellis Weismer S. Language deficits in poor comprehenders: a case for the simple view of reading. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006 Apr;49(2):278-93. — View Citation
Catts HW, Nielsen DC, Bridges MS, Liu YS. Early Identification of Reading Comprehension Difficulties. J Learn Disabil. 2016 Sep;49(5):451-65. doi: 10.1177/0022219414556121. Epub 2014 Oct 24. — View Citation
Chall, J. S. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Chall, J. S., & Jacobs, V. A. (2003). Poor children's fourth-grade slump. American Educator, 27, 14-15.
Ciullo S, Lo YL, Wanzek J, Reed DK. A Synthesis of Research on Informational Text Reading Interventions for Elementary Students With Learning Disabilities. J Learn Disabil. 2016 May-Jun;49(3):257-71. doi: 10.1177/0022219414539566. Epub 2014 Jun 23. — View Citation
Clements, D. H. (2007). Curriculum research: Toward a framework for research-based curricula. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 35-70.
Connor CM, Morrison FJ. Individualizing Student Instruction in Reading: Implications for Policy and Practice. Policy Insights Behav Brain Sci. 2016 Mar;3(1):54-61. doi: 10.1177/2372732215624931. Epub 2016 Jan 20. — View Citation
Coyne, M. D., McCoach, D. B., Loftus, S., Zipoli Jr, R., Ruby, M., Crevecoeur, Y. C., & Kapp, S. (2010). Direct and extended vocabulary instruction in kindergarten: Investigating transfer effects. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 3, 93-120.
Deniz F, Nunez-Elizalde AO, Huth AG, Gallant JL. The Representation of Semantic Information Across Human Cerebral Cortex During Listening Versus Reading Is Invariant to Stimulus Modality. J Neurosci. 2019 Sep 25;39(39):7722-7736. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0675-19.2019. Epub 2019 Aug 19. — View Citation
Dickinson, D. K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2010). Speaking out for language:Why language is central to reading development. Educational Researcher, 39, 305-310.
Douglas, K., & Albro, E. (2014). The progress and promise of the reading for understanding research initiative. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 341-355.
Durlak JA, DuPre EP. Implementation matters: a review of research on the influence of implementation on program outcomes and the factors affecting implementation. Am J Community Psychol. 2008 Jun;41(3-4):327-50. doi: 10.1007/s10464-008-9165-0. Review. — View Citation
ead-e Set Grow. (2013). Predictive assessment of reading: Pre-k to grade 3 edition. Clemmons, NC: Author.
Faggella-Luby, M. N., Drew, S. V., & Schumaker, J. B. (2015). Not such a simple story: Contradictory evidence from a review of story structure research for students at-risk. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 30, 61-75.
Fien H, Smith JL, Smolkowski K, Baker SK, Nelson NJ, Chaparro E. An Examination of the Efficacy of a Multitiered Intervention on Early Reading Outcomes for First Grade Students at Risk for Reading Difficulties. J Learn Disabil. 2015 Nov-Dec;48(6):602-21. doi: 10.1177/0022219414521664. Epub 2014 Feb 14. — View Citation
Fixsen, D., Naoom, S., Blase, K., Friedman, R., & Wallace, F. (2005). Implementation research: A synthesis of the literature. Tampa, FL: University of South Florida, Louis de la Parte Florida Mental Health Institute, National Implementation Research Network.
Foorman, B., Beyler, N., Borradaile, K., Coyne, M., Denton, C. A., Dimino, J., ... & Keating, B. (2016). Foundational Skills to Support Reading for Understanding in Kindergarten through 3rd Grade. Educator's Practice Guide. NCEE 2016-4008. National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance.
Gates, A. I., & MacGinitie, W. H. (2000). Gates-MacGinitie reading tests (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Riverside.
Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D., Coyne, M., Greenwood, C., & Innocenti, M. S. (2005). Quality indicators for group experimental and quasi-experimental research in special education. Exceptional Children, 71(2), 149-164.
Gottfredson DC, Cook TD, Gardner FE, Gorman-Smith D, Howe GW, Sandler IN, Zafft KM. Standards of Evidence for Efficacy, Effectiveness, and Scale-up Research in Prevention Science: Next Generation. Prev Sci. 2015 Oct;16(7):893-926. doi: 10.1007/s11121-015-0555-x. — View Citation
Hall MS, Burns MK. Meta-analysis of targeted small-group reading interventions. J Sch Psychol. 2018 Feb;66:54-66. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2017.11.002. Epub 2017 Nov 15. — View Citation
Harn BA, Linan-Thompson S, Roberts G. Intensifying instruction: Does additional instructional time make a difference for the most at-risk first graders? J Learn Disabil. 2008 Mar-Apr;41(2):115-25. — View Citation
Hebert, M., Bohaty, J. J., Nelson, J. R., & Lambert, M. C. (2018). Identifying and discriminating expository text structures: An experiment with 4th and 5th grade struggling readers. Reading and Writing, 31, 2115-2145.
Hogan TP, Adlof SM, Alonzo CN. On the importance of listening comprehension. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2014 Jun;16(3):199-207. doi: 10.3109/17549507.2014.904441. Review. — View Citation
Hogan, T. P., Bridges, M. S., Justice, L. M., & Cain, K. (2011). Increasing higher level language skills to improve reading comprehension. Focus on Exceptional Children, 44, 1-19.
Hulme C, Snowling MJ. Learning to Read: What We Know and What We Need to Understand Better. Child Dev Perspect. 2015 Mar 1;7(1):1-5. — View Citation
Institute of Education Sciences & National Science Foundation. (2013). Common guidelines for education research and development. Washington, DC: Authors.
Institute of Education Sciences. (2017, October). What Works Clearinghouse standards handbook (version 4.0). Retrieved August 17, 2018 https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/Docs/referenceresources/wwc_standards_handbook_v4.pdf
Jiang H, Logan J. Improving Reading Comprehension in the Primary Grades: Mediated Effects of a Language-Focused Classroom Intervention. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2019 Aug 15;62(8):2812-2828. doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-19-0015. Epub 2019 Aug 7. — View Citation
Joseph, L. M., Alber-Morgan, S., Cullen, J., & Rouse, C. (2016). The effects of self-questioning on reading comprehension: A literature review. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 32, 152-173.
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1997). Kaufman brief intelligence test (2nd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.
Language and Reading Research Consortium (LAARC), Farquharson K, Murphy KA. Ten Steps to Conducting a Large, Multi-Site, Longitudinal Investigation of Language and Reading in Young Children. Front Psychol. 2016 Mar 30;7:419. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00419. eCollection 2016. — View Citation
Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC), Jiang H, Logan JA, Jia R. Modeling the Nature of Grammar and Vocabulary Trajectories From Prekindergarten to Third Grade. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2018 Apr 17;61(4):910-923. doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-17-0090. — View Citation
Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC). Oral Language and Listening Comprehension: Same or Different Constructs? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017 May 24;60(5):1273-1284. doi: 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-16-0039. — View Citation
Language and Reading Research Consortium, & Chiu, Y. D. (2018). The simple view of reading across development: Prediction of grade 3 reading comprehension from prekindergarten skills. Remedial and Special Education, 39, 289-303. d
Language and Reading Research Consortium, Jiang, H., & Davis, D. (2017). Let's Know! Proximal impacts on prekindergarten through grade 3 students' comprehension-related skills. The Elementary School Journal, 118, 177-206.
Language and Reading Research Consortium. (2015). Learning to read: Should we keep things simple? Reading Research Quarterly, 50, 151-169.
Language and Reading Research Consortium. (2016). Use of the curriculum research framework (CRF) for developing a reading-comprehension curricular supplement for the primary grades. The Elementary School Journal, 116, 459-486.
Leslie, L., & Caldwell, J. S. (2011). Qualitative reading inventory (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.
Lohr, S., Schochet, P.Z., & Sanders, E (2014). Partially Nested Randomized Controlled Trials in Education Research: A Guide to Design and Analysis. (NCER 2014-2000). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Educational Research.
Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Schatschneider, C. (2018). Examining the simple view of reading with elementary school children: Still simple after all these years. Remedial and Special Education, 39, 260-273.
McNeish D, Stapleton LM. Modeling Clustered Data with Very Few Clusters. Multivariate Behav Res. 2016 Jul-Aug;51(4):495-518. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2016.1167008. Epub 2016 Jun 7. — View Citation
Moir, T. (2018). Why is implementation science important for intervention design and evaluation within educational settings? Frontiers in Education, 3.
Morris ZS, Wooding S, Grant J. The answer is 17 years, what is the question: understanding time lags in translational research. J R Soc Med. 2011 Dec;104(12):510-20. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.2011.110180. Review. — View Citation
Nation K, Adams JW, Bowyer-Crane CA, Snowling MJ. Working memory deficits in poor comprehenders reflect underlying language impairments. J Exp Child Psychol. 1999 Jun;73(2):139-58. — View Citation
Nation K, Clarke P, Marshall CM, Durand M. Hidden language impairments in children: parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language impairment? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2004 Feb;47(1):199-211. — View Citation
Nation, K. (2001). Reading and language in children: Exposing hidden deficits. The Psychologist, 14(5), 238-242.
Norbury CF, Gooch D, Wray C, Baird G, Charman T, Simonoff E, Vamvakas G, Pickles A. The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: evidence from a population study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016 Nov;57(11):1247-1257. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12573. Epub 2016 May 16. — View Citation
Pearson, P. D., & Gallagher, M. C. (1983). The instruction of reading comprehension. Contemporary educational psychology, 8(3), 317-344.
Petscher Y, Justice LM, Hogan T. Modeling the Early Language Trajectory of Language Development When the Measures Change and Its Relation to Poor Reading Comprehension. Child Dev. 2018 Nov;89(6):2136-2156. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12880. Epub 2017 Jul 5. — View Citation
Phillips BM, Tabulda G, Ingrole SA, Burris PW, Sedgwick TK, Chen S. Literate Language Intervention With High-Need Prekindergarten Children: A Randomized Trial. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2016 Dec 1;59(6):1409-1420. doi: 10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0155. — View Citation
Phillips BM, Zhao Y, Weekley MJ. Teacher language in the preschool classroom: Initial validation of a classroom environment observation tool. Early Educ Dev. 2018;29(3):379-397. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2017.1408371. Epub 2017 Dec 13. — View Citation
Piasta, S. B., Farley, K. S., Mauck, S. A., Soto Ramirez, P., Schachter, R. E., O'Connell, A. A., . . . Weber-Mayrer, M. (in press). At-scale, state-sponsored language and literacy professional development: Impacts on early childhood classroom practices and children's outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology.
Piasta, S. B., Logan, J. A. R., Groom, L. J., Zettler-Greeley, C. M., Bailet, L. L., & Lewis, K. (2019). Implementation of a small-group emergent literacy intervention by preschool teachers and community aides. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Quinn, D. M., & Kim, J. S. (2017). Scaffolding fidelity and adaptation in educational program implementation: Experimental evidence from a literacy intervention. American Educational Research Journal, 54, 1187-1220.
Redmond SM, Ash AC, Christopulos TT, Pfaff T. Diagnostic Accuracy of Sentence Recall and Past Tense Measures for Identifying Children's Language Impairments. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2019 Jul 15;62(7):2438-2454. doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-18-0388. Epub 2019 Jun 20. — View Citation
Ritchie SJ, Bates TC. Enduring links from childhood mathematics and reading achievement to adult socioeconomic status. Psychol Sci. 2013 Jul 1;24(7):1301-8. doi: 10.1177/0956797612466268. Epub 2013 May 2. — View Citation
Rudd, A., & Johnson, R. B. (2008). Lessons learned from the use of randomized and quasiexperimental field designs for the evaluation of educational programs. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 34, 180-188.
Sanetti, L. M. H. T. R. (2009). Toward developing a science of treatment integrity: Introduction to the special series. School Psychology Review, 38, 445.
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals:4. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
Silverman, R., & Crandell, J. D. (2010). Vocabulary practices in prekindergarten and kindergarten classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(3), 318-340.
Torgesen, J. K. (2000). Individual differences in response to early interventions in reading: The lingering problem of treatment resisters. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 15(1), 55-64.
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Vaughn, S., Linan-Thompson, S., Kouzekanani, K., Pedrotty Bryant, D., Dickson, S., & Blozis, S.A. (2003). Reading instruction grouping for students with reading difficulties. Remedial and Special Education, 24, 301-315.
Wright, T. S., & Cervetti, G. N. (2017). A systematic review of the research on vocabulary instruction that impacts text comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 52, 203-226
Wright, T. S., & Neuman, S. B. (2013). Vocabulary instruction in commonly used kindergarten core reading curricula. The Elementary School Journal, 113(3), 386-408.
* Note: There are 75 references in all — Click here to view all references
Type | Measure | Description | Time frame | Safety issue |
---|---|---|---|---|
Other | Nonverbal intelligence as assessed by the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test - Second Edition nonverbal intelligence (matrices) subtest | The nonverbal intelligence (matrices) subtest of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test - Second Edition measures children's ability to to solve new problems, perceive relationships and complete visual analogies without taxing language skills. The matrices subtest requires the child to point to the picture that would complete a picture or abstract design. Nonverbal intelligence will be tested as a moderator of treatment effects in analyses. | right before intervention starts | |
Other | Change in timed word and nonword reading, as assessed by the Test of Word Reading Efficiency - Second Edition | Change in timed word and nonword reading accuracy will be assessed by asking children to read as many words and then nonwords as possible in 45 seconds each, as measured by the Test of Word Reading Efficiency - Second Edition. Timed word and nonword reading will be tested as a moderator of treatment effects in the analyses as well as a distal treatment outcome. | right before intervention starts, right after intervention ends, one year post intervention, and 2 years post intervention | |
Other | Language comprehension skills, as assessed by the OWL Language Screener | Language comprehension skills are assessed by the The OWL Language Screener, which requires children to point to the one picture out of four pictures that best represents a sentence.This measure will be tested as a moderator of treatment effects in analyses as well as the measure to determine if the child has language weaknesses to qualify for treatment. | right before intervention starts | |
Other | Academic performance will be assessed by the Massachusetts or Ohio State Assessments of Reading | Children's academic reading performance will be assessed by the Massachusetts or Ohio State Reading Assessments to examine distal treatment outcomes on state assessments of reading | 2 years after intervention ends | |
Primary | Treatment unit 1 (fiction) vocabulary knowledge as assessed by the LARRC Unit Vocabulary Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) total correct | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit Vocabulary Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses children's definitional knowledge of the 8 vocabulary words taught in unit 1 (fiction) and results in a total score of definitional specificity with 2 points possible for each word for a total of 16 points possible. | approximately 6 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 1 treatment | |
Primary | Treatment unit 2 (animals) vocabulary knowledge as assessed by the LARRC Unit Vocabulary Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) total correct | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit Vocabulary Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses children's definitional knowledge of the 8 vocabulary words taught in unit 2 and results in a total score of definitional specificity with 2 points possible for each word for a total of 16 points possible. | approximately 12 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 2 treatment | |
Primary | Treatment unit 3 (earth materials) vocabulary knowledge as assessed by the LARRC Unit Vocabulary Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) total correct | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit Vocabulary Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses children's definitional knowledge of the 8 vocabulary words taught in unit 3 and results in a total score of definitional specificity with 2 points possible for each word for a total of 16 points possible. | approximately 18 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 3 treatment | |
Primary | Treatment unit 4 (folktales) vocabulary knowledge as assessed by the LARRC Unit Vocabulary Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) total correct | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit Vocabulary Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses change in children's definitional knowledge of the 8 vocabulary words taught in unit 4 and results in a total score of definitional specificity with 2 points possible for each word for a total of 16 points possible per CAM. | approximately 22 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 4 treatment | |
Primary | Change in vocabulary knowledge as assessed by the LARRC Target Word Knowledge | Change in vocabulary knowledge as assessed by the Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Target Word Knowledge measures which assesses definitional knowledge of a sampling of words taught across the 4 treatment units, in addition to several words not taught in the intervention. | right before intervention starts, right after intervention ends, one year post intervention, and 2 years post intervention | |
Primary | Change in breadth of vocabulary as assessed by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test 5th Edition (PPVT-5) | Change in the breadth of vocabulary will be assessed by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test 5th Edition (PPVT-5), a standardized measure of the number of words that a child should know compared to a nationwide sample of age-matched peers. Children point to one picture out of four that best represents a word spoken by the examiner. | right before intervention starts, right after intervention ends, one year post intervention, and 2 years post intervention | |
Primary | Treatment unit 1 (fiction) comprehension monitoring as assessed by the LARRC Unit 1 Comprehension Monitoring Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit 1 (fiction) Comprehension Monitoring Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses children's ability to detect inconsistencies in stories read to them and to recall a strategy to fix their comprehension breakdown. | approximately 6 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 1 treatment | |
Primary | Treatment unit 2 (animals) comprehension monitoring as assessed by the LARRC Unit 2 Comprehension Monitoring Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit 2 (animals) Comprehension Monitoring Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses children's ability to detect inconsistencies in stories read to them and to recall a strategy to fix their comprehension breakdown. | approximately 12 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 2 treatment | |
Primary | Treatment unit 3 (earth materials) comprehension monitoring as assessed by the LARRC Unit 3 Comprehension Monitoring Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit 3 (earth materials) Comprehension Monitoring Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses children's ability to detect inconsistencies in stories read to them and to recall a strategy to fix their comprehension breakdown. | approximately 18 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 3 treatment | |
Primary | Treatment unit 4 (folktales) comprehension monitoring as assessed by the LARRC Unit 4 Comprehension Monitoring Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit 4 (folktales) Comprehension Monitoring Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses children's ability to detect inconsistencies in stories read to them and to recall a strategy to fix their comprehension breakdown. | approximately 22 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 4 treatment | |
Primary | Change in child's ability to monitoring comprehension, as assessed by the LARRC Comprehension Monitoring Test | Change in comprehension monitoring as assessed by the Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Comprehension Monitoring Test, which assesses child's ability to detect inconsistencies in stories read to them and to recall a strategy to fix their comprehension breakdown from the strategies learned across all 4 treatment units. | right before intervention starts, right after intervention ends, one year post intervention, and 2 years post intervention | |
Primary | Treatment unit 1 (fiction) knowledge of text structures as assessed by the LARRC Unit 1 Text Structure Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit 1 Text Structure Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses children's ability to listen to a story and answer questions about the stories' structure, such as characters, settings, problem, and resolution. | approximately 6 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 1 treatment | |
Primary | Treatment unit 2 (animals) knowledge of text structures as assessed by the LARRC Unit 2 Text Structure Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit 2 Text Structure Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses children's ability to listen to an expository text passage and answer questions about the passage, such as main idea and important details. | approximately 12 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 2 treatment | |
Primary | Treatment unit 3 (earth materials) knowledge of text structures as assessed by the LARRC Unit 3 Text Structure Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit 3 Text Structure Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses children's ability to listen to an expository text passage and answer questions about the passage, such as main idea and important details. | approximately 18 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 3 treatment | |
Primary | Treatment unit 4 (folktales) knowledge of text structures as assessed by the LARRC Unit 1 Text Structure Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) | The Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Unit 4 Text Structure Curriculum-Aligned Measure (CAM) assesses children's ability to listen to a story and answer questions about the stories' structure, such as characters, settings, problem, and resolution. | approximately 22 weeks after start of intervention, at the end of Unit 4 treatment | |
Primary | Change in child's ability to use their knowledge to fill in the gaps in short stories, as assessed by the LARRC Inference Task | Change in child's ability to use their knowledge to fill in the gaps in short stories, as assessed by the Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) Inference Task, which assesses child's ability to answer questions about short stories they have heard presented by the examiner. | right before intervention starts, right after intervention ends, one year post intervention, and 2 years post intervention | |
Primary | Change in story retelling abilities, as assessed by the CUBED Narrative Retell measure | Change in child's ability to accurately and completely retell a story as assessed by the CUBED Narrative Retell measure. The examiner tells the child a story using pictures and the child retells the story back to the examiner. | right before intervention starts, right after intervention ends, one year post intervention, and 2 years post intervention | |
Primary | Change in ability to understand stories, as assessed by the Test of Narrative Language | Change in the child's ability to understand stories, as assessed by the Test of Narrative Language. The examiner tells the child stories and asks the child questions about those stories. | right before intervention starts, right after intervention ends, one year post intervention, and 2 years post intervention | |
Primary | Change in a child's ability to comprehend short passages read aloud to them, as assessed by the LARRC Listening Comprehension Measure | Change in a child's ability to comprehend short passages read aloud to them, as assessed by the LARRC Listening Comprehension Measure, which requires a child to listen to two short stories and answer questions about those stories | right before intervention starts, right after intervention ends, one year post intervention, and 2 years post intervention | |
Primary | Change in a child's ability to comprehend short passages they read, as assessed by the LARRC Reading Comprehension Measure | Change in a child's ability to comprehend short passages they read, as assessed by the LARRC Listening Comprehension Measure, which requires a child to read two short stories and answer questions about those stories | right before intervention starts, right after intervention ends, one year post intervention, and 2 years post intervention | |
Primary | Change in reading comprehension, as assessed by the norm-referenced Gates-McGinitie Reading Test | Change in child's ability to comprehend short passages that the child reads to themselves, as assessed by their ability to answer questions about those passages using the norm-referenced Gates-McGinitie Reading Test | right before intervention starts, right after intervention ends, one year post intervention, and 2 years post intervention |
Status | Clinical Trial | Phase | |
---|---|---|---|
Active, not recruiting |
NCT06062147 -
Impact of Teacher Training and the Introduction of Early Adjustments on the Developmental Trajectory of Children at Risk of SLLD in Pre-school.
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT05242575 -
The Influence of Immersive Virtual Field Trips on Academic Vocabulary
|
N/A | |
Enrolling by invitation |
NCT03838016 -
Preventing Speech and Language Disorders in Children With Classic Galactosemia
|
N/A | |
Recruiting |
NCT06250101 -
Training Grammar With Meaning
|
N/A | |
Active, not recruiting |
NCT05900180 -
Pediatric Speech Therapy Session Frequency and Speech Outcomes
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT05487521 -
EEG Changes in Pediatrics With Language Dysfunction Evaluation of Sleep EEG Changes in Paediatric Patients With Language Dysfunction: A Follow up Study. EEG Changes in Pediatrics With Language Dysfunction
|
Phase 3 |