Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Clinical Trial
Official title:
A Placebo-controlled Trial of Brain Activity Changes Following Psychostimulant Medication in Adolescent Combined-subtype ADHD
The purpose of this study is to examine the neural basis of response inhibition, working
memory, and sustained attention in adolescents and adults with
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), with particular emphasis on quantifying the
effects of methylphenidate (i.e., treatment with psychostimulants) on neural function.
Participants will undergo electrophysiological measurement of brain function during
laboratory cognitive tasks. This research is aimed to develop a better understanding of how
ADHD neural dysfunction relates to clinical presentation and medication response during the
transition from adolescence to adulthood. The specific aims and hypotheses are:
Specific Aim: To characterize the effect of Ritalin (methylphenidate) on neural activity
underlying performance on the response inhibition task in ADHD adolescents and adults.
Hypothesis 1) Methylphendiate will increase N2 and P3 amplitude in ADHD persons during
medicated EEG sessions; Hypothesis 2) There will be a significant age × medication
interaction such that ADHD teens will show increased amplitude of N2 while medicated,
particularly at frontal sites, whereas ADHD adults will show differentially greater effect
of medication on P3 amplitude and latency at central sites. Hypothesis 3) Brain activity
assessed by fMRI will differ between unmedicated and medicated states.
By conservative estimate, Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a common
disorder that occurs in at least 3 to 6% of school-aged children (Barkley, 1990). It is
marked by symptoms of inattention and hyperactive-impulsive behavior that are
developmentally inappropriate. These symptoms must occur in multiple settings and impair
functioning to a clinically significant degree (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association,
1994). ADHD is believed to be a developmental disorder that begins in early childhood, which
distinguishes it from disorders of attention and behavioral control that arise later as
consequence of brain injury. Although ADHD was once thought to affect only children,
considerable evidence has accumulated to show that ADHD frequently continues into adulthood.
Approximately 60% of ADHD youth (about 3-4% of all adults) have clinically significant
residual problems as adults (Faraone, 1999; Hechtman, 2000; Mannuzza & Klein, 1999).
Although inattention problems appear to persevere into adulthood unchanged, studies suggest
that some persons' ADHD hyperactive/impulsive symptoms diminish following teenage years.
However, the mechanism for this decrease is not currently known (Wender, 1987; Weiss &
Hechtman, 1986, 1993; Brown, 1995).
Expert consensus holds that a deficit in response inhibition is a core feature of ADHD
(Barkley, 1998). Supporting this, neuropsychological research suggests that ADHD persons'
commonly perform poorly on tasks that require inhibition of response. These tasks, called
Go/No-Go or Stop Signal tasks, require continual manual responding to successively presented
stimuli and an occasionally withheld response following a predefined signal. Numerous
neuropsychological studies have shown that ADHD children, teens, and adults respond more
variably on Go trials on such tasks. They also have longer reaction times and more failures
to inhibit responses on No-Go trials (Koshack et al., 2003; Oosterlaan, et al., 1998;
Wodushek & Neumann, 1999). Psychostimulant medications like methylphenidate improve
performance deficits (Bedard et al., 2003).
The link between abnormalities of brain structure/function and cognitive test performance in
ADHD persons is well-established, if poorly understood. There is evidence for smaller right
frontal lobe volume, abnormalities in basal ganglia size, and reductions in white matter
tracts in persons with ADHD (Castellanos et al., 1994; Castellanos et al., 1996; Filipek et
al., 1997; Hynd et al.,1993; Mataro et al., 1997). Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia are
involved in planning, initiation, execution, and supervision of ongoing cognitive and motor
functions (Fuster, 1997). Structural brain abnormalities correlate with deficits in these
cognitive functions and with impulsive behaviors that are symptoms of ADHD (Casey et al.,
1997; Mataro et al., 1997; Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2000). Functional imaging techniques,
which quantify neural systems operation with respect to cognitive processes, are
particularly valuable tools used to characterize dysfunctional neural networks. The most
frequently employed method is electrophysiology, in which small voltage changes on the scalp
occurring during task performance are recorded and averaged into waveforms that depict brain
function to events of interest. These average EEG waveforms are called event-related
potentials (ERPs). ERPs to Go/No-Go tasks are well characterized in healthy, non-clinical
samples (Logan, 1994). The most frequently observed component to No-Go trials is a large
negative potential occurring approximately 275 msec following stimulus presentation (N2)
that has a fronto-central distribution. The large N2 has been interpreted as a reflection of
early response inhibition processes. Electrophysiological studies find that the large N2
often seen with successful inhibition is diminished in ADHD children and adolescents
(Overtoom et al., 2002; Pilszka et al., 2000; Yong-Liang et al., 2000). A literature review
reveals no comparable studies of ADHD adults, so the degree of N2 abnormality in older ADHD
persons is not known.
ERP source localization suggests that the No-Go N2 arises from activity in inferior
prefrontal cortex (Bokura, 2001; Pliszka, 2000). The few FMRI studies of ADHD are consistent
with this finding (Garavan et al., 1999; Liddle et al., 2001; Watanabe et al., 2002). Rubia
et al. (1999) find less brain activity in right superior-lateral prefrontal cortex, right
inferior prefrontal cortex, and left caudate nucleus in unmedicated ADHD adolescent boys
relative to controls. Vaidya et al. (1998) report similar findings for ADHD children, and
also showed that methylphenidate increased activity in the striatum for ADHD participants.
These results support a fronto-temporal N2 decrease in ADHD in brain areas implicated by
structural studies as abnormal.
Durston and colleagues also found evidence that compared to non-ADHD, ADHD children also
activate a more diffuse network of posterior and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex regions to
subserve successful response inhibition (Durston et al., 2003). Increasing age also
significantly alters the pattern of activity seen on Go/No-Go tasks for healthy, non-ADHD
persons in these brain regions. Children activate more volume of prefrontal cortex compared
to adults (Casey et al., 1997). There also is evidence that healthy adults and teenagers use
different neural networks to achieve comparable response inhibition task performance (Rubia
et al., 2000). Healthy adults show modulation of ventral prefrontal activity depending on
task difficulty, whereas children appear to maximally activate these areas generally
(Durston et al., 2002). In a sample of ADHD participants ages 8 to 20, age was found to
correlate positively with left inferior frontal cortex, and correlate negatively with
hemodynamic activity in left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Tamm et al., 2002). In other
words, there was increasing use of cortex known to be a primary substrate of response
inhibition, while reliance on cortex that is a primary substrate of working memory and other
"on-line" cognitive processes decreased with age. These differences suggest that age is an
important variable in understanding response inhibition neural activity in both ADHD and
non-ADHD persons.
Age-related changes to behavior or cognitive functioning are typically interpreted within a
developmental framework. Researchers hypothesize that age-related improvements in neural
function (i.e., increased neural connectivity or functional specialization) are responsible
for improvements in ADHD hyperactive/impulsive symptomatology. A body of evidence indirectly
supports such contentions. Adults and adolescents are developmentally distinct with regard
to brain structure and function. Structural MR studies consistently show increases in white
matter volume throughout early childhood to early adulthood (Caviness et al.,1996; Giedd et
al., 1999) reflecting increasing connectivity among cortical areas, especially in reciprocal
connections between prefrontal and posterior brain areas (Goldman-Rakic, 1987). Gray matter
is seen to increase in a linear fashion until puberty and decrease during adolescence in
most brain regions. These curves peak in the frontal and parietal lobes at about age 12, the
temporal lobes at age 16, and the occipital lobes at increase until 20 (Giedd et al., 1999).
These findings are paralleled by histological animal and in vivo studies of human brain
development that show a sharp reduction in syntapogenesis at puberty, followed by an period
of synaptic pruning that continues at a similar rate throughout middle adulthood (Bourgeois,
2001). These changes presumably reflect the shaping of brain structure/function following
experience (Purves, 1988), and such changes likely reflect continuing performance
improvements on some cognitive tests throughout adolescence.
The juxtaposition of changes in brain structure/function and ADHD behavior from adolescence
to adulthood raises the possibility that the two might be linked. Furthermore, if these
factors are directly related, a link should be observable in a study that focuses on a core
impairment of ADHD (i.e., response inhibition) across different age groups. Two positions
have arisen in the field of developmental neurobiology to explain how ADHD symptoms
diminish. First, some researchers hold that brain areas crucial for behavioral inhibition
(i.e., ventral prefrontal cortex) may continue to mature, albeit at a delay. Second, it is
possible that other, unimpaired cognitive mechanisms develop to compensate for behavioral
inhibition system impairment. Among others, some candidate compensatory mechanisms include
stimulus evaluation or working memory processes. Because of the lack of research, neither of
these positions have great support at the current time; moreover, the two possibilities are
not incompatible. However, this question is crucial in understanding the pathophysiology of
ADHD. Figure 2 (right) depicts a two possible relationships of response inhibition
development in terms of ERP N2 amplitude. In Figure 2A, the diminished N2 seen in ADHD youth
could be hypothesized to resolve with increasing age. Alternatively (Figure 2B), there may
be no such gains in response inhibition N2 neural function. Rather, other cognitive
processes that can be measured with different ERP indices may reflect the development of
compensatory neural mechanisms in older ADHD persons, such as those reflected by the P3.
Summary Previous research shows that response inhibition measured on Go/No-Go tasks is
abnormal in all ages of ADHD persons, and that methylphenidate improves performance and
alters neural function. When electrophysiological indices of response inhibition (i.e., the
N2 ERP) are compared between No-Go success and failure trials, ADHD persons show differences
related to automatic processes involved with response inhibition thought to involve
prefrontal brain activity. This conclusion is supported by fMRI studies that localize No-Go
hemodynamic response to inferior lateral prefrontal cortex and medial cortex (i.e., anterior
cingulate and premotor cortex). Therefore, the N2 measures the strength of inhibitory neural
processing, likely reflecting activity in brain structures known to be abnormal in ADHD with
respect to both structure and hemodynamic function. However, electrophysiology has not yet
been used to measure behavioral inhibition in ADHD adults, or to compare ADHD adults and
youth.
This study will use electrophysiology to determine whether age-related differences in ADHD
symptom expression are related to differences in brain activity involved with response
inhibition. Furthermore, by quantifying ADHD brain function both on and off psychostimulant
medication, it will be possible to determine how age influences ADHD persons' neural
response to methylphenidate. We broadly hypothesize that methylphenidate will affect each
groups' ERPs differently. Medication-related differences also will be observed in ADHD
symptom severity and neuropsychological test performance. Innovations include that this will
be the first electrophysiology study 1) of response inhibition in ADHD adults, 2) that
attempts to directly link brain function changes to symptomatology changes in maturation,
and 3) that examines medication effects on cognition and behavior across age groups in ADHD.
;
Observational Model: Case-Crossover, Time Perspective: Prospective
Status | Clinical Trial | Phase | |
---|---|---|---|
Recruiting |
NCT06129396 -
Effects of Aerobic Exercise Intervention in Adolescents With Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT04779333 -
Lifestyle Enhancement for ADHD Program 2
|
N/A | |
Recruiting |
NCT05935722 -
Evaluation of a Home-based Parenting Support Program: Parenting Young Children
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT03148782 -
Brain Plasticity Underlying Acquisition of New Organizational Skills in Children-R61 Phase
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT04832737 -
Strength-based Treatment Approach for Adults With ADHD
|
N/A | |
Recruiting |
NCT04631042 -
Developing Brain, Impulsivity and Compulsivity
|
||
Recruiting |
NCT05048043 -
Development of a Game-supported Intervention
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT03337646 -
Evaluation of the Effect and Safety of Lisdexamfetamine in Children Aged 6-12 With ADHD and Autism
|
Phase 4 | |
Not yet recruiting |
NCT06406309 -
Settling Down for Sleep in ADHD: The Impact of Sensory and Arousal Systems on Sleep in ADHD
|
N/A | |
Not yet recruiting |
NCT06080373 -
Formulation-based CBT for Adult Inmates With ADHD: A Randomized Controlled Trial
|
N/A | |
Not yet recruiting |
NCT06454604 -
Virtual Reality Treatment for Emerging Adults With ADHD
|
Phase 2 | |
Completed |
NCT02477280 -
Effects of Expectation, Medication and Placebo on Objective and Self-rated Performance
|
Phase 4 | |
Completed |
NCT02911194 -
a2 Milk for Autism and Attention-deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT02555150 -
A Comparison of PRC-063 and Lisdexamfetamine in the Driving Performance of Adults With ADHD
|
Phase 3 | |
Completed |
NCT02473185 -
Effects of Expectation, Medication and Placebo on Objective and Self-rated Performance During the QbTest
|
Phase 4 | |
Completed |
NCT02390791 -
New Technologies to Help Manage ADHD
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT02829970 -
Helping College Students With ADHD Lead Healthier Lifestyles
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT02780102 -
Cognitive-Motor Rehabilitation, Stimulant Drugs, and Active Control in the Treatment of ADHD
|
N/A | |
Recruiting |
NCT04296604 -
Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) Neuromodulation of Executive Function Across Neuropsychiatric Populations
|
N/A | |
Recruiting |
NCT04175028 -
Neuromodulation of Executive Function in the ADHD Brain
|
N/A |