Eating Behavior Clinical Trial
Official title:
Examining the Role of Perceived Body Boundaries and Spatial Frame of Reference in the Mechanism of Action of a Body Scan Meditation in Reducing Emotional Eating: A Randomized Controlled Study
NCT number | NCT05223348 |
Other study ID # | McGillU |
Secondary ID | |
Status | Completed |
Phase | N/A |
First received | |
Last updated | |
Start date | December 9, 2021 |
Est. completion date | March 18, 2022 |
Verified date | August 2022 |
Source | McGill University |
Contact | n/a |
Is FDA regulated | No |
Health authority | |
Study type | Interventional |
Emotional eating, or overeating in response to emotions, is problematic because of its link to weight gain, obesity, and psychopathology such as bulimia and binge eating disorder. To date, a vast amount of research has studied the psychological processes that cause individuals to overeat in response to emotions in an effort to develop ways to help individuals reduce their emotional eating. The aim of the current project is to study two psychological processes that can potentially be positively influenced to improve well-being: perceived body boundaries and a person's spatial frame of reference. Particularly, the investigators will examine how perceived body boundaries and spatial frames of reference can be positively influenced through a body scan meditation and thereby improve emotional eating. Perceived body boundaries refers to the continuum along which the self is experienced, from a body-encapsulated entity that is separate from the surrounding world to a more diffuse entity that is more connected with others and the environment. Spatial frames of reference describes the region within one's perception, often based in the body and construed as the self, that may be experienced as egocentric, through a preoccupation with internal events, or as allocentric, with feelings of unity and interdependence with others and the environment. One way for individuals to experience more diffuse body boundaries and allocentric frames of reference is through a body scan meditation. In this practice, individuals are instructed to intentionally shift their attention to various parts of the body and to notice what happens without judging or reacting. Thoughts and emotions are briefly noted if they arise, and attention is shifted back to the body. Recent research has shown that when individuals practice the body scan meditation, individuals are likely to experience greater positive emotions, lower negative emotions, lower ruminations, and higher psychological wellbeing. In addition, research has shown that individuals are able to experience more diffuse perceived body boundaries and more allocentric frames of reference through a body scan meditation. Based on this work, the researchers predict that when emotional eaters practice the body scan meditation, emotional eaters will experience more diffuse body boundaries, more allocentric frames of reference, and lower ruminations, which could in turn reduce their negative affect and food cravings. The researchers will test this hypothesis by asking emotional eaters to complete questionnaires that measure perceived body boundaries, spatial frames of reference, ruminations, negative emotions, and food cravings before and after a body scan meditation. To ensure that any changes in these measures are due to the meditation, the researchers will compare these findings with emotional eaters who complete the same measures before and after a control listening task. The findings of the current study will be used to recommend the body scan meditation to support emotional eaters in regulating their emotions, cravings, and eating behaviors.
Status | Completed |
Enrollment | 81 |
Est. completion date | March 18, 2022 |
Est. primary completion date | March 18, 2022 |
Accepts healthy volunteers | Accepts Healthy Volunteers |
Gender | All |
Age group | 18 Years and older |
Eligibility | Inclusion Criteria: - Individuals who self-report as emotional eaters, indicated by a score of 3.25 or higher on the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien et al., 1986) Exclusion Criteria: - Self-reported eating disorders - Self-reported psychiatric conditions - More than 10 hours of formal meditation practice (e.g., sitting meditation) or retreat experience in the last one year |
Country | Name | City | State |
---|---|---|---|
Canada | McGill University | Montréal | Quebec |
Lead Sponsor | Collaborator |
---|---|
McGill University |
Canada,
Alberts HJ, Mulkens S, Smeets M, Thewissen R. Coping with food cravings. Investigating the potential of a mindfulness-based intervention. Appetite. 2010 Aug;55(1):160-3. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.05.044. Epub 2010 May 21. — View Citation
Alberts HJ, Thewissen R, Raes L. Dealing with problematic eating behaviour. The effects of a mindfulness-based intervention on eating behaviour, food cravings, dichotomous thinking and body image concern. Appetite. 2012 Jun;58(3):847-51. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.009. Epub 2012 Jan 10. — View Citation
Annesi JJ, Johnson PH, Tennant GA, Porter KJ, Mcewen KL. Weight Loss and the Prevention of Weight Regain: Evaluation of a Treatment Model of Exercise Self-Regulation Generalizing to Controlled Eating. Perm J. 2016 Summer;20(3):15-146. doi: 10.7812/TPP/15-146. Epub 2016 Feb 1. — View Citation
Austin JH. Zen-brain reflections. MIT press; 2010.
Christoff K, Cosmelli D, Legrand D, Thompson E. Specifying the self for cognitive neuroscience. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011 Mar;15(3):104-12. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.001. Epub 2011 Feb 1. — View Citation
Crosswell AD, Moreno PI, Raposa EB, Motivala SJ, Stanton AL, Ganz PA, Bower JE. Effects of mindfulness training on emotional and physiologic recovery from induced negative affect. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017 Dec;86:78-86. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.08.003. Epub 2017 Aug 4. — View Citation
Dahl CJ, Lutz A, Davidson RJ. Reconstructing and deconstructing the self: cognitive mechanisms in meditation practice. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015 Sep;19(9):515-23. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.001. Epub 2015 Jul 28. Review. — View Citation
Dambrun M, Berniard A, Didelot T, Chaulet M, Sylvie DV, Corman M, Juneau C, Lea MM. Unified consciousness and the effect of body scan meditation on happiness: alteration of inner-body experience and feeling of harmony as central processes. Mindfulness. 2019;10(8):1530-1544.
Dambrun M. When the dissolution of perceived body boundaries elicits happiness: The effect of selflessness induced by a body scan meditation. Conscious Cogn. 2016 Nov;46:89-98. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.09.013. Epub 2016 Sep 28. — View Citation
Deng Y-Q, Li S, Tang Y-Y. The relationship between wandering mind, depression and mindfulness. Mindfulness 2014;5(2):124-128.
Desbordes G, Gard T, Hoge EA, Hölzel BK, Kerr C, Lazar SW, Olendzki A, Vago DR. Moving beyond Mindfulness: Defining Equanimity as an Outcome Measure in Meditation and Contemplative Research. Mindfulness (N Y). 2014 Jan 21;2014(January). doi: 10.1007/s12671-013-0269-8. — View Citation
Farb NA, Segal ZV, Mayberg H, Bean J, McKeon D, Fatima Z, Anderson AK. Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007 Dec;2(4):313-22. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm030. — View Citation
Forman EM, Herbert JD, Moitra E, Yeomans PD, Geller PA. A randomized controlled effectiveness trial of acceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive therapy for anxiety and depression. Behav Modif. 2007 Nov;31(6):772-99. — View Citation
Forman EM, Shaw JA, Goetter EM, Herbert JD, Park JA, Yuen EK. Long-term follow-up of a randomized controlled trial comparing acceptance and commitment therapy and standard cognitive behavior therapy for anxiety and depression. Behav Ther. 2012 Dec;43(4):801-11. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2012.04.004. Epub 2012 Apr 24. — View Citation
Frayn M, Knäuper B. Emotional eating and weight in adults. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues. 2018; 37(4):924-933.
Gallagher I I. Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000 Jan;4(1):14-21. — View Citation
Gendall KA, Joyce PR, Sullivan PF. Impact of definition on prevalence of food cravings in a random sample of young women. Appetite. 1997 Feb;28(1):63-72. — View Citation
Hanley AW, Dambrun M, Garland EL. Effects of Mindfulness Meditation on Self-Transcendent States: Perceived Body Boundaries and Spatial Frames of Reference. Mindfulness (N Y). 2020 May;11(5):1194-1203. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01330-9. Epub 2020 Feb 14. — View Citation
Hanley AW, Garland EL. Spatial frame of reference as a phenomenological feature of self-transcendence: Measurement and manipulation through mindfulness meditation. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice. 2019; 6(4): 329-345.
Hanley M, Khairat M, Taylor K, Wilson R, Cole-Fletcher R, Riby DM. Classroom displays-Attraction or distraction? Evidence of impact on attention and learning from children with and without autism. Dev Psychol. 2017 Jul;53(7):1265-1275. doi: 10.1037/dev0000271. Epub 2017 May 4. — View Citation
James W. The self. The self in social interaction. 1968;141-49.
Jhangiani R, Tarry H, Stangor C. Principles of social psychology; 1st international edition.
Kabat-Zinn, Jon. Wherever You Go, There You Are: Mindfulness Meditation in Everyday Life. Hachette Books; 1994:104.
Ng HH, Wu CW, Huang FY, Cheng YT, Guu SF, Huang CM, Hsu CF, Chao YP, Jung TP, Chuang CH. Mindfulness Training Associated With Resting-State Electroencephalograms Dynamics in Novice Practitioners via Mindful Breathing and Body-Scan. Front Psychol. 2021 Oct 28;12:748584. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.748584. eCollection 2021. — View Citation
Price CJ, Hooven C. Interoceptive Awareness Skills for Emotion Regulation: Theory and Approach of Mindful Awareness in Body-Oriented Therapy (MABT). Front Psychol. 2018 May 28;9:798. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00798. eCollection 2018. — View Citation
Ricca V, Castellini G, Fioravanti G, Lo Sauro C, Rotella F, Ravaldi C, Lazzeretti L, Faravelli C. Emotional eating in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Compr Psychiatry. 2012 Apr;53(3):245-51. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.04.062. Epub 2011 May 26. — View Citation
Sauer-Zavala SE, Walsh EC, Eisenlohr-Moul TA, Lykins, EB. Comparing mindfulness-based intervention strategies: Differential effects of sitting meditation, body scan, and mindful yoga. Mindfulness. 2013; 4(4): 383-388.
Sevinc G, Hölzel BK, Hashmi J, Greenberg J, McCallister A, Treadway M, Schneider ML, Dusek JA, Carmody J, Lazar SW. Common and Dissociable Neural Activity After Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction and Relaxation Response Programs. Psychosom Med. 2018 Jun;80(5):439-451. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000590. — View Citation
Yaden DB, Haidt J, Hood RW, Vago DR, Newberg, AB. The varieties of self-transcendent experience. Review of General Psychology. 2017;21(2):143-160.
* Note: There are 29 references in all — Click here to view all references
Type | Measure | Description | Time frame | Safety issue |
---|---|---|---|---|
Other | Adherence Checks | The adherence checks will be adapted from the mind wandering check used in Dambrun et al. (2019). Participants will be asked two questions: to indicate the estimated percentage of time (from 0 to 100%) they were preoccupied by thoughts of the past or future; and to indicate the percentage of time (from 0 to 100%) they were able to follow the instructions of the practice recording. | Immediately after the intervention | |
Other | Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness- 2 | The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness-2 is a 32-item multidimensional measure that assesses key aspects of the mind-body interaction, namely, interoceptive awareness. Respondents indicate on a 6-point Likert-type scale (from 0 = never to 5 = always) how much each statement applies to them. The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness-2 has eight scales: noticing of body sensations, not distracting from negative (e.g., painful) sensations, not worrying about uncomfortable sensations, sustaining attention to sensations, awareness of the link between sensations and emotions, regulating psychological distress via attention to sensations, active listening to the body for insight, and experiencing one's body as safe and trustworthy. A higher score would mean individuals have a higher level of interoceptive awareness (better score), and a lower score would mean individuals have a lower level of interoceptive awareness (worse score). | Up to 2 days before testing | |
Other | Equanimity Scale | Equanimity is defined as an even-minded mental disposition towards all objects of experience regardless of their affective valence or source. This scale is to measure equanimity and contains 12 items assess the "even-minded state of mind," and the other 13 items measure "hedonic independence". The participants will use a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never or very rarely to 5 = very often or always) for this scale. A higher score would mean an individual has a more even-minded mental disposition towards all objects of experience (better score) and a lower score would mean an individual has less even-minded mental disposition towards all objects of experience (worse score) | Up to 2 days before testing | |
Other | Drexel Defusion Scale | Cognitive defusion is defined as the ability to gain psychological distance from thoughts and feelings by seeing them as mere events in the mind. Participants are asked to indicate the degree to which they would be able to defuse from hypothetical situations with negative thoughts or feelings on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from "not at all" (0) to "very much" (5). A higher score would indicate a greater ability to defuse from negative thoughts and feelings (better) and a lower score would indicate an individual is less able to defuse from negative thoughts and feelings (worse) | Up to 2 days before testing | |
Other | Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire | The five factor mindfulness questionnaire is a 39-item questionnaire in which respondents indicate on a 5-point Likert-type scale (from 1 = never or very rarely true to 5 = often or always true) how much various statements describe them. This questionnaire aims measures five facets of habitual or trait mindfulness: Observing, Describing, Acting with Awareness, Non-Judging of Internal Experiences, and Non-Reactivity to Inner Experiences ("I perceive my feelings and emotions without having to react to them"). A higher score would indicate a greater score of trait mindfulness, indicating greater ability to observe, describe, act with awareness, be non-judgmental of internal experiences, and non-reactive to inner experiences. A lower score indicates a lower level of all these indices (a worse score). | Up to 2 days before testing | |
Primary | Negative affect subscale of the Positive and Negative Affect Scale | Negative affect will be measured using the negative affect subscale of the Positive and Negative Affect Scale. The lowest score is 10, and the highest score is 50. The higher outcome would mean a worse outcome as this indicates higher negative affect, and a lower score would indicate a better outcome (lower negative affect). | The investigators are assessing change from baseline (immediately after the mood induction) to immediately after the intervention. | |
Primary | Desire subscale of the Food Cravings Questionnaire-State Version | The intensity of food cravings will be measured using the Desire subscale of Food Cravings Questionnaire- State Version. Three items that target cravings for food from the Desire subscale of Food Cravings Questionnaire- State Version will be used in the current study. Specifically, participants rate the three items: "I am craving tasty food", "I have an urge for tasty food", "I have an intense desire to eat tasty food." All items will be rated on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The mean score on the three items will be used as the food craving measure. | The investigators are assessing change from baseline (immediately after the mood induction) to immediately after the intervention. | |
Secondary | Perceived Body Boundaries Scale | Perceived body boundaries are described as the degree to which the self is experienced as a discrete, body-encapsulated entity, separate and isolated from the surrounding world. Participants are asked to rate the strength of their perceived body boundary between self and the world using a 7-point Likert type scale (1 = weak boundary, 7 = strong boundary). Scores are ranked from 0.0 to 15.5, and higher scores indicate higher salience in perceived body boundaries (better score), and lower scores indicate lower salience in perceived body boundaries (worse score). | The investigators are assessing change from baseline (immediately after the mood induction) to immediately after the intervention | |
Secondary | Spatial Frame of Reference Continuum | Spatial frame of reference is defined as the region within one's perception, often based in the body and construed as the self, that is involved in the sense of connection between self, others, and the environment | Immediately after the mood induction and immediately after the intervention | |
Secondary | Brief State Rumination Inventory | Rumination involves repetitively focusing attention on the causes and implications of one's negative mood. | Immediately after the mood induction and immediately after the intervention |
Status | Clinical Trial | Phase | |
---|---|---|---|
Completed |
NCT03117374 -
Impact of Web-based School Nutrition Intervention to Increase Fruits, Vegetables and Dairy
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT04025099 -
Internal Cues Versus External Cues for Eating and Activity
|
N/A | |
Recruiting |
NCT06111040 -
Nurturing Needs Study: Parenting Food Motivated Children
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT05485168 -
Combined Effects of Sequential Variety and Portion Size on Meal Intake of Women
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT03241121 -
Study of Eating Patterns With a Smartphone App and the Effects of Time Restricted Feeding in the Metabolic Syndrome
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT03850990 -
Effect of Gut-Cued Eating on BMI and Efficacy of Open-Label Placebo to Augment Weight Loss
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT02470949 -
Influence of a Monopoly Game on Subtle Behaviors
|
N/A | |
Recruiting |
NCT01863212 -
The Role of the FTO Gene in Reward System Activation in Obese and Healthy Subjects
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT02729675 -
Innovative Approaches to Increase F&V Intake Thru Worksites
|
Phase 2 | |
Completed |
NCT05405244 -
Examination of Bromocriptine on Homeostatic and Hedonic Mechanisms of Food Intake in Individuals at High Risk for T2DM
|
Phase 3 | |
Completed |
NCT04971811 -
Effects of Energy Density on Self-served Snacks in Preschool Children
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT05019872 -
Al Dente or Well Done? The Eating Rate of a Pasta Meal Modified by Texture
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT04605224 -
Effectiveness of a Culinary Class on Food Literacy and Eating Behaviours of Francophone High School Students
|
||
Not yet recruiting |
NCT06448117 -
Mind Nutrition Study: Nutrition in the Prevention of Depressiveness and Sustaining Work Ability
|
N/A | |
Recruiting |
NCT04526743 -
Eating Behavior and Weight Trajectory After Bariatric Surgery
|
||
Active, not recruiting |
NCT05026411 -
Food Reward Circuit Change by Orthodontics
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT05173311 -
Pilot Study: The Effectiveness of a Mobile Application in Increasing Vegetable Acceptance
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT05149066 -
#KindGirlsInACTion: A Programme for the Promotion of Mental Health of Female Adolescents
|
N/A | |
Completed |
NCT03779321 -
Effect of Food Acceptability on Appetite Hormones' Response in Normal Weight vs. Obese Male Subjects
|
N/A | |
Recruiting |
NCT06108128 -
Food for Thought: Executive Functioning Around Eating Among Children
|
N/A |